Published: Oct. 1, 2012 By

 Stephen Collector/The New York Times/Redux

Associate Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience Tor Wager, director of CU Boulder’s Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Lab. Photo: Stephen Collector/The New York Times/Redux

What you don’t know won’t hurt you, goes the old canard, but what you believe can make a difference when it comes to pain relief, and not just in a subjective way. When you expect that a drug or placebo will relieve pain, and it does, it’s not simply a matter of fooling your brain.

That’s one lesson from intriguing new research co-authored by Associate Professor of Psychology and Neuroscience Tor Wager, director of CU Boulder’s Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Lab.

“A placebo can cause you to believe something and that can have very deep effects on the emotional brain. … We are being fooled, led to have positive expectations, but that generates positive effects not just in the subjective sense but in how our brain functions,” Wager says of “Dissociable Influences of Opiates and Expectations of Pain,” published in the June issue of The Journal of Neuroscience.

In two studies, Wager and his research partners from New York University, Columbia University and the University of Wisconsin, Madison examined the relationship between placebo and an opioid analgesic in physiological brain responses while manipulating the expectations of test subjects.

“To account for psychological factors, clinical trials assume that placebo effects are independent of drug effects and can be ‘subtracted away to reveal the true drug effect,’” the authors write.

But, Wager says, the studies suggest that may not be the case, and that placebo-drug interactions — including expectations of relief — might work on different neurobiological processes and therefore have “additive” effects.

“In fact, (opioids and placebos) affect different things in your brain when you use them side by side,” he says. “There is something about what we believe in treatment that isn’t simply duplicated by the drug effect.”

The work has implications for the way clinical drug trials are conducted. He offers the example of a patient who is so drugged by the administration of opioids that she “does not have the cognitive machinery to generate expectations.” In that case, you wouldn’t see an additive benefit of the placebo.

Wager describes beliefs or expectations as a way of anticipating future events, to which both brain and body respond.

“If you just won the lottery and Ed McMahon came to your house with the check, you haven’t got the money yet. But your entire body responds to the anticipation of having the money,” he says. The same can be true of any expectation. “We tend to think of beliefs as inconsequential, but the brain evolved to respond to them profoundly.”

Does that mean any and all beliefs cause actual physiological responses in the brain? Not necessarily, Wager says. Many people, he says, capitalize on the placebo effect, such as those who perform “energy work.”

“Unless we can find plausible physiological mechanisms, I think we’ll find a lot of (the response to these techniques) might be due to expectation,” he says.

See a profile of Tor Wager in The New York Times here. See Tor Wager’s thoughts paired with those of other leading experts in Now Toronto here.